TITLE

Postural control in strabismic children versus non strabismic age-matched children

AUTHOR(S)
Lions, Cynthia; Bui-Quoc, Emmanuel; Bucci, Maria
PUB. DATE
September 2013
SOURCE
Graefe's Archive of Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology;Sep2013, Vol. 251 Issue 9, p2219
SOURCE TYPE
Academic Journal
DOC. TYPE
Article
ABSTRACT
Background: Several studies have shown that achieving a dual task modifies postural control; however, their results are conflicting. The goal of the present study is to compare the effect of a simple task (eye fixation) to the effect of a dual task (saccadic eye movements) on postural balance in strabismic and in non strabismic children. Methods: Postural stability was recorded with a platform (Techno Concept©) in ten strabismic children aged from 5.4 to 13.8 years (mean age: 8.8 ± 2.5 years). Data were compared to that of ten age-matched non strabismic control children. We analyzed the surface area, the length and the mean speed of the center of pressure (CoP). Results: Strabismic children are more instable than control children. The surface (340 mm), the length (402 mm) and the mean speed (16 mm/s) of the CoP are significantly higher in strabismic children than in control children (160 mm, 280 mm and 11 mm/s, respectively). In addition, both strabismic and control children have a better postural control during saccades than during a fixation task. Surface, length and mean speed of the CoP are significantly reduced during the saccade task (214 mm, 306 mm and 12.5 mm/s, respectively) than during the fixation task (285 mm, 376 mm and 14.8 mm/s, respectively). Conclusions: Abnormal postural control in strabismic children could be due to their visual deficits. The postural improvement observed in a dual task (saccades) vs. a simple task (fixation) might be due to the fact that postural control becomes more automatic during saccadic eye movements.
ACCESSION #
90132820

 

Related Articles

  • Infantile Esotropia: An Inevitable Legacy? Sarubbi, Maria Cristina; Letiz, Maria Alejandra // American Orthoptic Journal;2005, Vol. 55, p112 

    Introduction and Purpose: To investigate the clinical signs and electrooculographic (EOG) records in first-degree relatives of patients with infantile esotropia. Subjects and Method: Eight families were randomly selected with at least one member with Ciancia syndrome, a common form of infantile...

  • Visual Exploration during Locomotion Limited by Fear of Heights. Kugler, Günter; Huppert, Doreen; Eckl, Maria; Schneider, Erich; Brandt, Thomas // PLoS ONE;Aug2014, Vol. 9 Issue 8, p1 

    Background: Visual exploration of the surroundings during locomotion at heights has not yet been investigated in subjects suffering from fear of heights. Methods: Eye and head movements were recorded separately in 16 subjects susceptible to fear of heights and in 16 non-susceptible controls...

  • Transsaccadic Perception of Saccade Target and Flanker Objects. Germeys, Filip; De Graef, Peter; Verfailie, Karl // Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception & Performan;Aug2002, Vol. 28 Issue 4, p868 

    Assesses the proposed dual route model to account for location-dependent and location-independent preview benefits in transsacadic object perception. Use of episodic object and long-term memory representations across a saccade; Application of saccade-contingent display change techniques;...

  • Biologically inspired calibration-free adaptive saccade control of a binocular camera-head. Bruske, Jörg; Hansen, Michael; Riehn, Lars; Sommer, Gerald // Biological Cybernetics;1997, Vol. 77 Issue 6, p433 

    Abstract. This paper describes fast and accurate calibration free adaptive saccade control of a four-degrees-of-freedom binocular camera-head by means of Dynamic Cell Structures (DCS). The approach has been inspired by biology because primates Face a similar problem and there is strong evidence...

  • Binocular coupling in chameleon saccade generation. Frens, M.A.; van Beuzekom, A.D.; Sándor, P.S.; Henn, V. // Biological Cybernetics;1998, Vol. 78 Issue 1, p57 

    Abstract. Chameleons are capable of making a saccade with one eye while the other does not move. This virtually unique feature poses questions regarding the organization of the saccadic system of the chameleon. By comparing real data with a simulated test signal, we studied whether the saccade...

  • Abnormal Saccadic Eye Movements in Autistic Children. Kemner, C.; Verbaten, M. N.; Cuperus, J. M.; Camfferman, G.; van Engeland, H. // Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders;Feb1998, Vol. 28 Issue 1, p61 

    The saccadic eye movements, generated during a visual oddball task, of autistic children, normal children, children with attention deficit disorder and hyperactivity (ADDH), and dyslexic children were examined to determine whether autistic children differed from these other groups in saccadic...

  • Superior colliculus encodes distance to target, not saccade amplitude, in multi-step gaze shifts. Bergeron, Andre; Matsuo, Satoshi; Guitton, Daniel // Nature Neuroscience;Apr2003, Vol. 6 Issue 4, p404 

    The superior colliculus (SC) is important for generating coordinated eye-head gaze saccades. Its deeper layers contain a retinotopically organized motor map in which each site is thought to encode a specific gaze saccade vector. Here we show that this fundamental assumption in current models of...

  • Going for the goal. Krauzlis, Richard J.; Carello, Christopher D. // Nature Neuroscience;Apr2003, Vol. 6 Issue 4, p332 

    Focuses on a study on saccadic eye movement which suggests that during the programming of orienting movements a population of neurons monitor the progress towards the final goal of the movement. Role of superior colliculus in controlling saccadic eye movements; Neuronal activity in the superior...

  • Functional imaging of the primate superior colliculus during saccades to visual targets. Moschovakis, A. K.; Gregoriou, G. G.; Savaki, H. E. // Nature Neuroscience;Oct2001, Vol. 4 Issue 10, p1026 

    The primate superior colliculus (SC) is a midbrain nucleus crucial for the control of rapid eye movements (saccades). Its neurons are topographically arranged over the rostrocaudal and mediolateral extent of its deeper layers so that saccade metrics (amplitude and direction) are coded in terms...

Share

Read the Article

Courtesy of THE LIBRARY OF VIRGINIA

Sorry, but this item is not currently available from your library.

Try another library?
Sign out of this library

Other Topics