TITLE

Language Development and Symbolic Play in Children With and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia

AUTHOR(S)
Lyytinen, Paula; Poikkeus, Anna-Maija; Laakso, Marja-Leena; Eklund, Kenneth; Lyytinen, Heikki
PUB. DATE
August 2001
SOURCE
Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research;Aug2001, Vol. 44 Issue 4, p873
SOURCE TYPE
Academic Journal
DOC. TYPE
Article
ABSTRACT
The purposes of this study were to investigate (a) whether children in families with a positive history of dyslexia were more likely to show delays in language development than children without family risk and (b) whether a delayed onset of expressive language (late talking) predicted later language development. We analyzed the language development of 200 children longitudinally at 14, 24, 30, and 42 months and assessed their symbolic play at 14 months. Half of the children (N = 106) were from families with a history of dyslexia (the Dyslexia Risk [DR] group), and other children served as age-matched controls. Parental reports and structured tests were used to assess children's receptive and expressive language and symbolic play. No differences emerged between the two groups in receptive language, symbolic play, or on the Bayley MDI. The groups, however, diverged in expressive language measures. The maximum sentence length at 2 years and object naming and inflectional morphology skills at 3.5 years were higher for the control group than for the DR group. Reynell receptive score at 2.5 years provided the greatest unique contribution to the prediction of the children's receptive and expressive language. Children's risk status did not contribute to receptive language, but provided a significant contribution to their expressive language at 3.5 years, even after the variance associated with parental education and children's previous language skills was controlled. Late talkers in the DR group differed from the other members of the DR group in both receptive and expressive language at 3.5 years, although in the control group children with a late-talking history performed at age-level expectations. The findings suggest that children with a familial risk for dyslexia and with a history of late talking are at higher risk for delays in language acquisition than children without the familial risk for dyslexia.
ACCESSION #
5813565

 

Related Articles

  • Symbolic Play in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Stanley, Gillian C.; Konstantareas, M. Mary // Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders;Aug2007, Vol. 37 Issue 7, p1215 

    The relationship between symbolic play and other domains, such as degree of autistic symptomatology, nonverbal cognitive ability, receptive language, expressive language, and social development, was investigated. The assessment files of 101 children with Autism Spectrum Disorder were studied....

  • The relationship of language and symbolic play in children with hearing loss. Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine; Snyder, Lynn S.; Day, Diane // Volta Review;Summer98, Vol. 100 Issue 3, p135 

    Examines the relationship between symbolic play and language development in a sample of children ages between eight months and 36 months who are deaf and hard of hearing. Aspects of symbolic play behavior that are highly related to language development; Usefulness of the Play Assessment...

  • Dyslexia in Children. Myklebust, Helmer R.; Johnson, Doris // Exceptional Children;Sep1962, Vol. 29 Issue 1, p14 

    The article focuses on the study of the process of learning to read as an integral part of total language development in the U.S. The author points out that the relationship between the auditory and the read word is being manifested also through the study of children who have dyslexia. The study...

  • HELPING YOUR CHILD TO TALK.  // Australian Parents;Jun/Jul2000, Issue 116, p37 

    Provides tips in helping children in the development of speech and language skills.

  • bilingual babbling. Sethi, Anita // Baby Talk;Oct2011, Vol. 76 Issue 8, p19 

    The article discusses ways on how to teach one's child a second language which include learning together, being consistent, and taking one word at a time.

  • "Me Talk!".  // Parenting Early Years;Feb2012, Vol. 26 Issue 1, p26 

    The article stresses that the first words of a child will happen around 18 to 24 months.

  • 'What to Expect'. Murkoff, Heidi // Baby Talk;Jun/Jul2003, Vol. 68 Issue 5, p20 

    Answers a question about the language development in a child whose caregiver primary speaks Spanish. Reason that some parents choose a caregiver who speaks another language; Research finding on the ability of the children before age five; Consequence of exposing the child early on to two languages.

  • Speech and language.  // Children & Young People Now (Haymarket Business Publications Ltd;1/9/2008, p37 

    The article lists ways in fostering children's speech and language development in children's centers and schools which include getting the attention of the child through interaction, making eye contact and introducing activities for learning a language that are fun and exciting.

  • "Me Talk!". D. S. // Parenting School Years;Feb2012, Vol. 26 Issue 1, p26 

    The article explains the language development in children, from bubbling to using two-word phrases.

Share

Read the Article

Courtesy of VIRGINIA BEACH PUBLIC LIBRARY AND SYSTEM

Sorry, but this item is not currently available from your library.

Try another library?
Sign out of this library

Other Topics