TITLE

The Intersection of the Development of Gestures and Intentionality

AUTHOR(S)
Crais, Elizabeth; Douglas, Diane Day; Campbell, Cheryl Cox
PUB. DATE
June 2004
SOURCE
Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research;Jun2004, Vol. 47 Issue 3, p678
SOURCE TYPE
Academic Journal
DOC. TYPE
Article
ABSTRACT
This study examined the development of deictic and representational gestures in 12 typically developing children from 6 to 24 months of age. Gestures were categorized into J. Bruner's (1981) 3 broad (and 8 specific) communicative functions: behavior regulation (i.e., requesting objects, requesting actions, protesting), joint attention (i.e., commenting, requesting information), and social interaction (i.e., representational gestures, attention seeking, social games). Ongoing parental completion of researcher-created gesture recording forms and monthly researcher observational confirmation were used to capture the emergence and consistent use of targeted gestures. Within each specific functional category, a hierarchy of development was documented for the gestures and behaviors used to signal that intent This study provides rich detail as to the order of emergence of common deictic and representational gestures and their relationship to other preceding and concomitant behaviors that children use to signal their intentions. Furthermore, the results document younger ages of emergence, in comparison with previous studies, for most of the targeted gestures and provide insight into the controversy in the literature regarding the relative emergence of declarative and imperative gestures.
ACCESSION #
14084250

 

Related Articles

  • Talking From Hand to Mouth. Begley, Sharon; Hayden, Thomas // Newsweek;3/15/1999, Vol. 133 Issue 11, p56 

    Focuses on research on how chimpanzees and deaf babies communicate with hand gestures. Why researchers believe that the human brain is wired for gestural communication; The use of American Sign Language; The question of how and when language originated; How deaf children can invent...

  • Contact between Mexican Sign Language (LSM) and American Sign Language (ASL) in two Texas border areas. Quinto-Pozos, David Gilbert // Sign Language & Linguistics;2004, Vol. 7 Issue 2, p215 

    The article highlights the findings of a study, which described the contact between the Mexican sign language and the American sign language in two deaf communities in Texas. The study described the language production of eight deaf individuals who participated in one-on-one interviews and group...

  • How language evolved from manual gestures. Corballis, Michael C. // Gesture;2012, Vol. 12 Issue 2, p200 

    Several lines of evidence suggest that human language originated in manual gestures, not vocal calls. These are the ability of nonhuman primates to use manual action flexibly and intentionally, the nature of the primate mirror system and its homology with the language circuits in the human...

  • A comparative analysis of animals' understanding of the human pointing gesture. Miklósi, Ádam; Soproni, Krisztina // Animal Cognition;Apr2006, Vol. 9 Issue 2, p81 

    We review studies demonstrating the ability of some animals to understand the human pointing gesture. We present a 3-step analysis of the topic. (1) We compare and evaluate current experimental methods (2) We compare available experimental results on performance of different species and...

  • Three fingers point back at you. Nyerges, Christopher // Christian Science Monitor;8/10/98, Vol. 90 Issue 179, p9 

    Recounts how the author learned to be more supportive of his fellow teachers. The older teacher who taught him the importance of encouragement; What the author learned about pointing at someone and why it was important not to.

  • You must see the point: Automatic processing of cues to the direction of social attention. Langton, Stephen R.H.; Bruce, Vicki // Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception & Performan;Apr2000, Vol. 26 Issue 2, p747 

    Explores the processing of pointing gestures comprising hand and combined head and gaze cues to direction. Moderation of cross-modal interference effect of pointing hand gestures by gesturer's head-gaze orientation; Bidirectional within-interference effect of hand and head cues; Rationale for...

  • Selection of Touch Gestures for Children's Applications: Repeated Experiment to Increase Reliability. Nor Azah Abdul Aziz; Nur Syuhada Mat Sin; Batmaz, Firat; Stone, Roger; Wai Hing Chung, Paul // International Journal of Advanced Computer Science & Application;Apr2014, Vol. 5 Issue 4, p97 

    This paper discusses the selection of touch gestures for children's applications. This research investigates the gestures that children aged between 2 to 4 years old can manage on the iPad device. Two experiments were conducted for this research. The first experiment was carried out in United...

  • Gesture Development: A Review for Clinical and Research Practices. Capone, Nina C.; McGregor, Karla K. // Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research;Feb2004, Vol. 47 Issue 1, p173 

    The aim of this article is to provide clinicians and researchers a comprehensive overview of the development and functions of gesture in childhood and in select populations with developmental language impairments. Of significance is the growing body of evidence that gesture enhances, not...

  • Person-Centred (Deictic) Expressions and Autism. Hobson, R. Peter; García-Pérez, Rosa M.; Lee, Anthony // Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders;Apr2010, Vol. 40 Issue 4, p403 

    We employed semi-structured tests to determine whether children with autism produce and comprehend deictic (person-centred) expressions such as ‘this’/‘that’, ‘here’/‘there’ and ‘come’/‘go’, and whether they understand...

Share

Read the Article

Courtesy of VIRGINIA BEACH PUBLIC LIBRARY AND SYSTEM

Sorry, but this item is not currently available from your library.

Try another library?
Sign out of this library

Other Topics